Next >>
Regardless of the convoluted pondering over covert/overt operations
concerning the Subject here (e.g., Agrs, Spec-TP, mult-Spec, etc.), the overall
structure nonetheless must project a partially-fledged IP (via
TP). In other words, since the T component of IP (in the Pollockian
sense) has already been established at this stage-2, the reduced
VP-stage (as seen in stage-1) can't suffice. This is the crucial
difference between my stage-1 (where no IPs were reported at
all) and stage-2 (where the acquisition (albeit not the mastery)
of IP was spotted. In sum, this has the flavor of saying that
although all feature operations may have their initial
locus at LF, only in the sense that procrastinate seems
to prefer covert operations to overt ones, there is an added
stipulation which states that once an IP projects overtly at
PF (creating a functional checking domain), all subsequent clauses
thereafter must theoretically project an IP (albeit minimally
via TP). Again, if Agr were to have no strong features at LF,
PF considerations would give no reason for it to be present
at all (Chomsky op.cit: 351). Therefore, the stipulation of
T suffices to force us into projecting an IP for (10 e-j), even
though the relevant features/categories of the clause seemingly
project a VP. The distinction of duty between overt and covert
movements may be expressed by the fact that overt movements
need to carry along whole categories for PF convergence (i.e.,
there is no sense describing a covert PF as there is no feature
strength distinction having to do with phonological features),
while in covert movement features raise alone. Hence, once an
XP has been acquired (at PF) by the onset of the category <x>
being carried, the established XP is in place and avoids vacuous
projections at all expense.
(The Roles of Agr vs. T ) The roles between
Agr and T can be further reduced. Recall that the majority of
early Nominative Case usages found were among ambiguously marked
finite/nonfinite structures. Among such early constructs, a
minus-Tense/nonfinite interpretation makes the most sense. There
are two clear reasons for this. Firstly, at the onset of verbal
3sg Nom Case constructions, the present tense marker "s"
is left out (as in 10). (See §1.2.3 for Tense). Secondly, in
Double-Verb (SVV) constructions, e.g., Infinitive constructions
with a Nom Case, the infinitive particle "to"--which
arguably has anaphoric/infinitival tense and is matrix-bound
by INFL--is always missing. (In fact, the infinitive particle
"to" doesn't seem to emerge until late in File
23 (3;2); however, when it is used, the matrix clause shows
proper tense). An argument can be made that when there is no
"to", there must also be no tense in the matrix
INFL to bind T in the complement clause. In other words, although
Agr(eement) is realized on the Nom subject of these Double-Verb
constructions, the main verb's T(ense) is assumed not to project--much
in the same manner as the finite particle 'to' is assumed
not to project. Consider such early examples of [-T/+Agr] SVV
constructions with mostly ambiguous finite main verbs:
(12) Nominative SVV [+Agr/-T] with
Infinitive "to" omission:
a. I want ř kick (file
8: 2;4) d. She going ř touch my man (3;2)
b. I want ř cook (file
14: 2;7) e. You want ř help me?(file 22: 3;0)
c. I want ř hit a spider (file 22:
3;0) f. I want ř write (file 24: 3;4)
(Nb. There are no counter
examples of an apparent Tense bound matrix clause without a
"to" infinitive complement: e.g., He wanted/wants
go home, etc., is not attested in my data. Such examples
falsify my argument here that "to" has tense
and binds tense to its matrix INFL-clause.)
Let us embark on Schütze and Wexler's (1996), Schütze's (1997)
discussion that seeks to analyze (inter alia) the Present
Tense "s" suffix as unambiguously signaling
the presence of tense and agreement. Such a description
would predict Accusative subjects never to occur alongside the
suffix "s". E.g., Wexler points out that the
combination "Him cries" is unattested. However,
in my own data, and in a wide array of literature found elsewhere
(e.g., Huxley (1970), Aldridge (1989), among others), such combinations
are in fact reported. One interesting way, though, in which
we could save Wexler's elaborate paradigm would be to suggest
that the English suffix "s" doesn't mutually
signal T and AGR, but rather exclusively signals Tense--the
suffix "s" would then have no overt bearing
on Agreement at all (i.e., AGR simply remains indifferent to
the suffix "s" due to the nature of an invisible
agreement assigning mechanism in English (e.g.,
He/She cry [+Agr, -T], Schütze et al. op.cit: 9), though
it may coincidentally sit among the presence of the Tense marker
suffix "s" for 3sg.[i]
Consider the revised suffix "s" paradigm below:
(13)
Suffix "s"=> I.
+Marks Tense [ii]
(Radford: lectures '97)
a. Him cries (-Agr)
b. I works (+Agr invisibly marked)
II. -Marks
Agreement: (+Agr invisibly marked)
c. (He cry)
d He crie-s- ([+T], "s" doesn't mark Agr)
III. +Marks
Tense (+Agr invisibly marked)
e. He cries
Furthermore, following Radford, if we
assume that some children have the following entries for subsequent
inflections:
(14) a. + d if past tense
b. +s if present tense
c. ř otherwise (perhaps
as a universal default)
and, if we assume (contra Schütze: 1997) that T and Agr are
not fused together and optionally projected at PF, we would
then expect to find the following paradigm of early utterance
types (features in brackets are those features carried by the
Verb/INFL):
(15) Non-Fused T/Agr
Paradigm
a. I/He cried =>[+T,+Agr]
e. Me/Him cried =>[+T,-Agr]
b. I cry => [-T*, +Agr]
f. Me/Him cries =>[+T,-Agr] [iii]
c. Me cry => [-T*, -Agr] d.
I/He cries =>[+T, +Agr]
(* no indication of Tense--used
both in past/present contexts)
Noting in (13) above the phonological sameness of II and
III with regards to cries (ex. d, e)--viz., it remains
impossible to tell in English whether or not only Agreement
projects (as opposed to both agreement and tense) regarding
the suffix "s". However, what we gain by postulating
example (c) in II (illustrating no potential correlation between
the suffix "s" and +AGR) is an added feature
in I which now can indicate a potential T(ense) without Agr(eement).
In treating the suffix "s" in such a restrictive
manner, a feasible account can now be made showing how a T feature
("s") could go omitted, whilst Nominative Case
via invisible Agreement is maintained [-T, + Agr]: e.g.,
He get a bat, He do it, etc. (cf. (10) above). (Here,
Agr-features on I are checked by the Nominative Subject, et
vice versa). Theoretically speaking, the converse then holds
with respect to utterances containing [+T, -Agr]: e.g., Him
goes, Him cries, Him is hiding, me walked, me broke, etc[iv]--all
violating the D-I correction (cf. 1a)--though Wexler argues
against this. (Wexler makes the distinction that only the "s"
suffix--and not the "ed" suffix--is associated
with person/number features as well as tense.) Nevertheless,
we may wish to claim here that while the "s", "ed"
specifically mark Tense, they fail to signal any Agreement--prompting
rather a Default Agreement marker (Radford: lectures '97).
Similarly, examples of an overgenerated/default suffix
"s", may likewise be interpreted in ways which
attribute "s" particularly to T only and not
Agr (though Agr is correctly marked by the Nom Subject in ex.
a-c) (p.c. Radford). Consider the following token examples of
the first emergence of "+S " (file 23) found
in my Data:[v]
(16)
a. I works (file 23: 3;2) g.
Where is you? (cf. §1.2.3-Table 1.8)
b. I hurts (file 24: 3;3) h. Here is me (file
24)
c. I makes (file 25: 3;6) i. You
is done (file 24)
d. Him cries (file 25) j. You is no nice
(file 24)
e. Him is hiding (file 25) k. This is your
books (file 25)
f. Him not (file 25) l. Him is my friend
(file 25)
(16')

An additional point to make here--giving further empirical
support to the above claim--is that we observe "s"
as only marking T and not Plural Agreement--i.e.,
the plural marking "s" on the Noun (book)
escapes verbal Agreement (are) (cf.16'k). The suffix
"s" in such examples (albeit few in number)
might be interpreted as rendering an overgeneralization effect
to that described above: specifically, while the suffix "s"
doesn't project its proper person/number Agr-features--e.g.,
rendering instead an impermissible 1sg Nominative (cf. ex. a-c)
(in ways similar to e.g., Him cries)--it does however
project a sort of default present tense. An overgeneralization
of the "s" may be construed in the light that
the present tense suffixes on 1,2sg/1,2,3pl main verbs must
be represented by a null constituent [ř]. The fact that
Nominative Case is assigned nevertheless under such a confused
state, I think, goes to the heart of the issue that the suffix
"s" plays only an overt unitary role
and not a dual role for the child (i.e., it exclusively marks
tense)--with the assumption that Agreement may be marked incidentally
by an invisible agreement mechanism (in English): (e.g.,
I/You/She/He/We/They hate syntax).
In sum, it is clear at this stage that the child has acquired
the +Interpretable Tense feature of "s", so
[+s] is used whenever INFL has [+T present]. (Unlike the adult
specification that calls for "s" iff +3Per-Sing-Pres,
the child's entry of "s" only refers to the
feature present tense and may not initially adhere to
person.) But INFL may optionally project Agr features as well.
Whether or not INFL projects an Agreement feature [present +Agr]
or [present -Agr], [+s] will continue to be used regardless
of Agr. The fact that the child may have access to Agr--as signaled
by the case of the subject--speaks only to the notion of the
Agreement mechanism itself as cited above. Moreover, the earlier
observation that certain aspects of Tense may actually be acquired
earlier then Agr, I think, reinforces the previous notion put
forward that children may first acquire (semantic) +Interpretable
features (Tense) of the suffix "s" and only
later do they come to acquire its -Interpretable features (Agreement).
This Discontinuity between the child-adult grammars may stem
from this notion that only +Interpretable features come on-line
at the earliest OI-stage. In this sense, the improper "s"
in (16') is restricted to Tense and thus doesn't involve itself
with the (invisible) Nominative assignment [+ Agr].
Recall at the end of (§1.1), we discussed the possibility that
IP may split into two merger operations--merger at PF and merger
at LF. The above analyses of "s" as a Tense
feature here further adds empirical support to that notion:
examples (cf. 10) a. He cut the tree, c. He
get a bat, d. He do it all demonstrate an AGRsp
[=assigning Nom case] merging at PF while TP [=Tense operator]
is covertly functioning at LF. Again, one crucial advantage
for categorizing "s" here as strictly a Tense
feature (and not a hybrid of T&Agr) is that we can achieve
clear-cut derivations of T and Agr merger operations at either
level of PF/LF. As an interesting side-note (pointed out to
me by Radford p.c.) these assumptions jibe nicely with what
we know about "South-Western British English"
which overgeneralizes the suffix "s" throughout
the present-verbal paradigm: (cf. I/We/You/She/They hates
syntax (Radford 1997ms ch. 10: p7)).
1.2.3 Possessive Structures
The following section is two-prong in nature. (i) Regarding
a stage-1, I claim that this initial stage manifests no inflections
whatsoever: contra Wexler, I find no clear evidence of Optionality.
Following suit, I then compare and contrast the data (Tables
1.4 & 1.5)--serving as a means to illustrate this Non-Inflectional
Stage-1 vs. an Optional Inflectional Stage-2. (ii)
In acknowledging a Wexlerian Optional-Stage for our second stage,
I put aside general issues of Optionality and proceed to shed
empirical light on the generally held hypothesis that No
Correlation necessarily holds between D and I outside
the appropriate checking domain (e.g., there should be no
correlation say between a lower Object-DP and INFL).
(Previews) Stage-1 (see Data section
below) suggests a Prefunctional Stage (viz., an early
stage which preexists underspecifications). N+N (=Genitive)
constructions are used alongside sporadic usage of (non-verbal
clause DPs) My/Mine. (The usage of the definite determiner
The is fully productive from the earliest files (cf.
§1.2.1)). Early usage of the determiners My and Mine
may indicate that these Poss(essive) Nom(inals) are, in fact,
Analogous to the robust early (file 1-7) usage of D(P)
The (=Analogical-The)--i.e., they comprise of
Default Case without their formal features of Genitive Case
and don't incorporate verbal elements.[vi]
Hence, all DPs (alike) at this stage would possibly mark for
+Def(initeness) only. In this sense, the young child may freely
alternate between Analogical-The and My/Mine for
the following type of logical expression: e.g., [DP
D [+Def] +N]; as in The-My/Mine
book, etc. (This amounts to saying that there is no clear-cut
distinction or reference of Possession for the child at this
initial stage-1.) There is some preliminary evidence to suggest
that the child's early (overgenerated) use of e.g., Mine's
(found in my corpus and elsewhere in the literature) may indeed
be accounted for in such a manner (cf. Radford 1990: 108ff)
[DP Mine [D 's] N book]].[vii]
In short, while The-My/Mine examples are attested
early on, we would not predict the overt morphological marking
of Possessive 's for this stage--whereas such marking
would be a clear indication of the acquisition of Case/Agreement
morphology. (Nb. This is empirically borne out--the first
signs of the productive usage of possessive 's come
in the very latest files. (See the section on Stage-2)
(Theory) Let's pick-up on Hoekstra et al's
observation of a D-I correlation (cf. §1.1). Although Hoekstra
et al. take the Definiteness Feature of the Subject as
the specific feature deficit responsible for the unspecification
of DP (Number being utilized as the main deficit of D
leading to null subjects/underspecification of IP), we can naturally
expand this notion of Definiteness to the +Agreement feature
in Pronominal Possessive D(P)s. This extension is made feasible
by Abney's (1987) seminal work which argues that possessive
nominals are in fact DPs Headed by a null determiner which carries
the formal Uninterpretable/+Agr(eement) property. Thus, following
Radford (class lectures 1997), (and keeping with the spirit
of Abney (ibid.)) a specified DP phrase in (17) below could
have one of the following two structures (17a/b) (17c illustrating
either the non-functional VP-stage ( i.e., [řAgr]
) or the underspecified IP-stage (i.e., [-Agr]
):
(17)
(a) [DP John's [D [+Agr]] book] [viii]
(cf. Radford: 1997)
(b) [DP John [D 's [+Agr]] book]
(c) [DP John [D Ř [ř/-Agr]] book]
(17')

We henceforth follow Chomsky (1995: 261) and favor the structure
in (17b) over (17a)--where the possessive 's positions
within the Head of D where it checks its Agreement properties.
In fact, the checking of possessive 's here is an anomaly
of sorts. For instance, Radford (p.c.) has come up with the
interesting idea that 's must somehow check its personless
features with the nominal in Spec. Since Nominals don't
typically carry case (cf. the non-contrast of Johna, Johnb
and Johnc--potentially
deriving--Nominativea,
Accusativeb
and Genitivec respectively) it's not at all straightforward
what kind of case we are considering here. Well, suppose that
one way of accounting for the ungrammaticality of e.g., *My's
book/Him's book/Her's book etc. (all of which show 's
to be in Head of D) is to say that the possessive 's
can't attach to a Pronominal with +Agreement/Case. In other
words, this amounts to saying that 's checks for Nominal
(default) personless-case features (e.g. John [-Per/-Agr]) and is exclusively associated with
non pronominals. In a paradoxical sense, +Agr (cf. 17b) might
actually mean the formal checking-off of an -Agr personless
feature.
We draw our attention here to the utterance John book
(=Gen) (cf. 17c) regarding the prefunctional stage-1 below.
In (17c), the Head of DP is vacuous. The above token example
is taken from my stage-1 and typify this structure. The claim
made here is that while early (nonspecified) DPs contain a Specifier
and a Complement, their Heads can be void of any morphological
material. The reasoning behind the claim that a DP projects
here--as opposed to the more traditional NP-analysis for N+N
(=Gen) constructions at this stage--is two fold in nature. (i)
Firstly, (cf. §1.1) we wish to suggest a two tier class of DPs:
a DP>VP (which has a lexical categorial status), and a DP>IP
(which has a functional status). (ii) Secondly, the DP-analysis
here jibes with the earlier observation that DPs may be initially
miscategorized as having lexical category status (cf. Radford:
1990). Recall we were able to account for the overgeneralization
of e.g., Mine's book etc. in this way (see note. 20).
Furthermore, we may wish to take a hard stance (following Abney)
and suggest that once the determiner The is acquired
(cf. §1.2.1), a DP must project. Consider the following token
examples taken from the prefunctional VP-stage found in my data
(files 2-7):
(18)

In the above structure, the Agr feature is [řAgr] absent altogether.
However, since +Def projects (the feature being closely associated
with Determinacy), the overall DP projects. Such default DPs
at this VP-stage match--though for different reasons--their
counterpart DPs within adult small clauses--where the Verb in
the matrix clause formally assigns Objective Case (via ECM):
(18')
a. I consider [sc=daddy's truck a safe
vehicle]
b. She doesn't want [sc=the truck in the garage]
c. I'll have [sc=my truck/mine looking clean]
d. I believe [sc=*he/him worthy of the post]
The above amounts to saying that although the child, at the
VP-stage, matches the adult skeletal structure of DPs, she fails
to realize any of the formal (-Interpretable/Case) feature specifications
of the Head (see note 20). This claim suggests that DPs (i.e.,
Determinacy) may emerge on the scene at the very earliest stage
of language development--triggered by the +Definiteness feature.
Hence, categorial features along with +Interpretable nominal
features (viz., semantically based f-features) are immediately accessible to the
child. Case properties [+Agr/-Intrepretable] on the other hand
are out of limits for the child at the prefunctional stage--Case
can only emerge once the functional IP-stage has been acquired
carrying along with it the likes of formal/abstract properties.
We are claiming that Subject/Object DPs indeed emerge as an
adult skeletal structure at the VP-stage but that there are
no specific formal properties attached to the Head D that have
to do with checking per se. None of the Determiners in
(18) contain their appropriate Case--all of their claims on
(Objective) Case are similarly derived via Default. To summarize,
the cited Possessive Nominals (DP>VP) are specified as follows:
(19)
Poss.Nom. Det Default Case Mis-specification
Example
a. The => +Objective
-Nominative The truck go.
b. Daddy (N+N)=> +Objective -Genitive Daddy
truck...
c. My/Mine => +Objective -Genitive My
truck...
d. Me => +Objective -Genitive Me
truck...
e. Him => +Objective -Genitive Him
truck...
We can recapitulate the above distribution of Objective case
by addressing the intrinsic asymmetry found between Subjects
and Objects. The notion that only the Subject is effected by
Hoekstra et al's D-I correlation (cf. §1.1) adds further support
to this asymmetry found between Subjects and Objects--namely,
the clausal positioning of these two DPs seems to be of some
relevance. This has prompted me to reconsider the idea that
apparent Accusative DPs (in Object position) do not necessarily
abide by the same constraints as their Subject/Nominative DP
counterparts. For instance, Chomsky has suggested that the case
of a given Expletive (e.g., there) would depend on its
counterpart associate DP within the given clause--E.g., expletive
there in the sentences:
(20)
a. There is a book on the shelf;
(cf. Chomsky 1995: 288)
b. There arrived yesterday a visitor from England;
c. I expected [there to be a book on the table]--
would take on its appropriate Case via its associated DP within
the respective clause:
(20')
a'. DP is...(DP=Nominative);
b'. DP arrived...(DP=Nominative);
c'. I expected [DP to be...] (DP=Accusative).
(There is clear evidence in the data that (i) such
asymmetries exist and (ii) that such asymmetries arise
in nature from syntactic deficits. Recall that in §1.2.2.1,
cf. Table 1.3, it was discussed that Accusative Subjects (via
default) were acquired much earlier than their Nominative counterpart.)
The above observations could be expanded to say that a DP in
Object position is of a very different category (regarding aspects
of features) than say a DP in Subject position, even though
the lexical entry is apparently the same. I take this difference
to be of a lexical vs. formal kind respectively--the
difference having nothing to do with the lexical item (per
se) but rather having everything to do with the item's feature
specification. Specifically speaking, a DP--(i) either
at a given VP-stage or (ii) infixed within a VP-projection
of an otherwise underspecified IP-stage--could theoretically
exist without its formal Agreement features being intact, and
carry only Objective case (via default). Hence, at the VP-stage,
a Subject D(P) may theoretically hold exactly those same feature
specifications as its Object D(P) counterpart--viz., only those
+Def/phi-features apply--making it difficult to tell whether
the D-I Correlation is even operative. (The notion of an Objective
DP in Subject position eventually leads to the complete breakdown
of the correlation regarding Subj/Obj-DPs in relation to INFL.)
(Data: Stage-1) As cited above, my data suggests
there to be a prefunctional stage where only N+N (=Genitive)
constructions and Caseless (clause fragment) My/Mine
DPs appear. This first stage, more generally, preexists any
form of (Under)specification regarding an IP (i.e., Tense and/or
Agreement): there is in all actuality No IP-projection. One
means of describing this stage is to call on the notion of The
Lexical Deficit Analysis (=LDA) (cf. Schütze: 1997).
The LDA basically states that at an early stage of language
development, the child may entirely miss out on e.g., Case markings,
Inflections, Agreements simply due to the fact that the child
has yet to acquire the specific lexical entry (as in morphologically
rich languages) or feature (as in English) associated with Case.
An altered tactical approach to LDA would be to assume that
although the child has indeed acquired the lexical entries for
e.g., Possessive DPs e.g., My/Mine, such entries lack
properties of case specification and, thus, are to be considered
as completely different (lexical) entries altogether--as opposed
to their +Agr case marked counterparts My/Mine: (similar
to the DP>IP/VP distinctions of The drawn-on earlier
in this chapter). (Though, of course, the two sets are homophonic,
we shall take the latter [+Agr] to be case marked only when
we have sufficient evidence--taken from other means--that Case
has been acquired. The essential criterion for determining (Gen)
case here is the acquisition of possessive 's
coupled with other factors regarding the acquisition of e.g.,
Nom/Acc Case, etc.) This amounts to the important observation
that lexical entries are defined by their bundle-of-features
(some features being acquired later than others). The DPs cited
below (Table 1.4) mark Head features as [-Agr/+Def] and never
fluctuate between [+/-Agr]. Evidence in support of the [-Agr]
deficit comes from the observation that Agreements associated
with Nominative Subjects, Possessive 's, and Finite
Verb constructions +s--forming the "benchmark"
criteria as mentioned above--never manifest at Stage-1 (§1.2.2.1-Table
1.2 for statistics on Nom/Acc Case). Compare and Contrast
the Tables (cf. 1.4, 1.5 and 1.6) illustrating a bi-model pattern
of Inflectional Acquisition:
(21)
Table 1.4 Stage-1 (files 2-8:
1;10-2;4)
Early Possessors: VP-Stage
[-Agr] Token Examples: Stage-1
a. N+N(Gen) n.=86
a'. Daddy truck. Mama bottle. Nicolas turn.
b. Pron.Poss n.=13 b'. My
cat. Me turn. Me pen. Mine bottle.
*My/Mine
*(My/Mine=restricted to clausal DPs)
c. Poss.'s n.=0/86
(n. 86=obligatory contexts)
d. His/Her n.=0/0
(no obligatory context: files 2-8)
e.. Det. The n.=100+
e' (see §1.2.1 for examples)
(cf. My/Mine= "Analogical-The")
(21')

(21") Token Examples (cf. Table
1.5 above)
a. I want me bottle. Where me
Q-car? That me car. Have me show.
b. Mine pasta, My pasta, I want my key. It is my t.v.
Where is my book?
c. No you train, It's you pen. It's you kite. It you
house?
d. Where's your friend?, Close your eyes. It's your
car? I got yours
e. I want to go in him house, Him bike is broken. It's
him house.
f. What's his name (x3)
The Data drawn from these two tables (above) seem
to suggest that Inflectional (Agreement) is gradually acquired
until the mastery threshold establishes itself at around
3:0 years: (cf. R. Brown's 90% criteria). The distinction made
here between early "Analogous-The-My/Mine constructs
(cf. Table 1.4) and those herein above (Table 1.5) is
that the early types never incorporate Verbal elements and are
said to be analogous to the early spotted Determiner The
(among the very first files)--both DPs The and My/Mine
here mark for [+Def] only and are said to be unspecified for
Agr. It is clear that at the later stage, where IPs are formed
which incorporate Agreements, that My/Mine examples now
do have access to proper Genitive Case.
Regarding the early usage of The and "Analogous-The"-My/Mine,
the two predominate schools-of-thought would characterize their
typical structures based on the above examples as follows:
(22)

In (22i), the DP correlates both its Internal
D-Spec feature [+Agr] (My= +Gen) and External I-Head
feature as [+Agr] (cat= +Nom), whilst seemingly in the
absence of any other overt specification for INFL. Tense is
unspecified and Agreement may or may not be specified, depending
on the account you accept here. (Whatever the case, no overt
factors present themselves in such a way as to force a +Agr
specification for (22i) outside of theory internal reasons.)
We take it that the most parsimonious structure is clearly that
of (22ii) where there is no overt need to project +Agr since
the DP might just as well be considered as Objective [-Agr/+Def]
(carrying the default case). In this sense, the DP is not +Gen
though the two structures phonetically resemble each other (see
note 20).
Our claim here is that stage-1--where there is
no evidence otherwise for INFL--we follow in the spirit of Radford
(ms1997) and suggest that overt DPs (in subject/object position
alike) may be forced into taking default Acc case since there
are no functional categories that could possibly host the moved
element driven by checking. In the sense of (22ii), there is
no established checking relation within DP just as there is
no checking relation within IP: since Agr/T are inert, they
have no need to enter into a checking relation. (In the words
of Radford (1990: 103), My/Mine are imposters.) We take
the Analogy (cf. 21c) that the Determiner The can equate
to the pronominal possessive My as a natural extension,
given the fact that they both seem to encode purely semantic/+Interpretable
features at this stage-1. In the sense of (22ii), The/My
pertain to +Interpretable (phi) features having to do with informal
semantics of e.g., definiteness/determinacy, and have nothing
whatsoever to do with more formal/abstract features such as
Case. Hence, the distribution and inter-relation between overt
(Pro)nominals can be summarized as follows (Radford: class lectures
'97):
(23)
a. Nominative if in a checking relation
to [+Agr] I (=> IP-Stage)
b. Genitive if in a checking relation to [+Agr] D
(=> IP-Stage)
c. Objective [ř/-Agr] (via default) otherwise
(=> VP-Stage)
It is clear from this distribution that the early
The-My/Mine examples above can't be considered as having
true Genitive case since they are considered here as carrying
the Default Objective case (cf. 23c).
(Interim Summary of Stage-1)
One interesting observation that we can deduce from the data
thus far is that a correlation, (pointed out to me by Andrew
Radford), seems to hold regarding the general acquisition of
a wider range of Inflection types. Radford (Radford & Galasso:
ms.1998) makes the observation that a (previously unreported)
symmetry holds between (i) the development of
Subject-Verb structures on the one hand and (ii)
Possessive Nominal structures on the other. That is,
the data seem to describe an initial (Stage-1) grammar purely
based on a Non-Inflectional-Prefunctional Paradigm. The nature
of the correlation, (a correlate which clearly speaks to the
protracted nature of Inflectional acquisition), could be claimed
as being governed-programmed by maturational factors--i.e.
functional categories (in this case INFL) are acquired (somewhat)
simultaneously given that once the brain can perceive and generalize
such formal categories, the "pandora's box" of functionalism
opens thus letting-in (potentially all-at-once) all previously
blocked formal aspects of grammar. In this sense, Radford's
(1990) original thesis that claims for a functional correlation--i.e.,
that functional categories DP, IP/CP (as a whole) embody a qualitative
kind and thus should be triggered to come "on-line"
simultaneously, (in a given child), once those factors of maturation
are in place--seems to ring true. For clarity, let's recap the
general claims being made here:
(i) The Data suggest an initial Stage-1 showing (inter
alia) no signs of the Inflection +S on the 3Pr/Sg
Verb, no signs of Possessive 'S on the Nominal Possessor,
and, more generally, no Subject-Agreement (where the subject,
if case marked, acquires the default Accusative setting).
(ii) The Data shows a symmetry illustrating parallel development
of +Agr/Inflections Possessive 'S and Verbal (excluded
here are potential stereo-type early Copula constructs (see
§1.2.3 copula) 3sg-pres +S, restated in Table
(1.6):
(24) Table 1.6
Development of Inflection
Occurrence in Obligatory Contexts
Age
3sg-pres +S*
Poss 'S
2;3-3;1
0/69 (0%) 0/118 (0%) (=>
VP: Stage-1)
3;2-3;6
72/168 (43%) 14/60 (23%) (=> IP/Optional:
Stage-2)
(* 0 of
69 (3sg-pers +S) indicates "true" copula/verbal
counts cf. Table 1.9--excluding the early 9 counts which were
assumed to be of a stereo-typic nature. Poss. +S cf. Tables
1.4 & 1.5)
I believe an underlying and central idea behind Radford's claim
can be proposed: specifically being, that a more general composition
of Agreement (acting in either Nominal or Verbal domains) seems
to be acquired in unitary fashion for the child. In other words,
the child's realization of [+Agr] for the two utterance types:
e.g. (i) Pat's cough and (ii) Pat's coughing might be
identically encoded by the child as +Agr--independent
of whether the Agreement mechanism itself is functioning nominally
or verbally (respectively). Radford utilizes Kayne (1994: 105)
to suggest that the formal aspects of the two Infections (cited
above) are one-of-a-kind and both pertaining to IP:
(25)
[IP Pat [ [+Agr nom] 's] cough] (=> [+Agr] Nominal)
b. [IP Pat [ [+Agr verb] 's] coughing] (=> [+Agr] Verbal)
c. [D/P0 [IP John ['s [car]]]] (cf. Kayne 1994: 105)
>In this sense, Kayne reduces the generalities behind the two
cases of Inflection to a common ancestral Agreement--viz., both
inflections ultimately governed by IP (25c).
(Data: Stage-2) Insofar that we acknowledge an
OI-Stage (Stage-2), we put aside theoretical issues and focus
rather on empirical content. This section examines if potential
correlations emerge between specified vs. undespecified
verbal IPs and relevant agreement vs. nonagreements (respectively)
regarding Subject/Object D(P)s. The interest lies in seeing if
this Developmental Symmetry (as reported above) holds any
further correlates for the OI-Stage. Intuitively, one would expect
that Spec-features, say within an Object-DP, are independent of
Spec-features within IP regardless of underspecification. Namely,
the assumed D-I correlation (Hoekstra et al.) is expected to hold
configuration only between the Head-features of a (Subject) D(P)
and that of its Specifier-features of INFL. However, as witnessed
in Kayne's treatment of an "overriding" IP-based Inflection
for Verbs and Nouns alike, and coupled with Radford's observation
above linking the two relevant inflections to a single "onset-time"
in acquisition, matters surrounding configurational dependencies
may not be so entirely straightforward. For instance, overall
residual effects of underspecification--given that Agr-O and Agr-S
are, in theory, essentially composed of the same formal material
[+Agr]--might conceivably be spotted "up-and-down" an
underspecified tree structure. (Chomsky (1995: 174) asserts that
formal phi-features of Agr equally pertain to Agr-O/Agr-S since
distinctions between Subject-S/Object-O labeling here are considered
as mnemonic devices.) The purely hypothetical notion I am playing-on
runs as follows: iff Agreement suffers a general deficit (i.e.,
[-Agr]) in one phrase of a sentence, say Object within VP, then
the Subject within IP, via extension, likewise instantiates
some deficit.
The examples below are merely geared to dispel any scenario
for an inter-phrasal IP correlate: showing Kayne's Agr-based
accounts of Verb/Nominal S-Inflection don't invalidate traditional
hypotheses for independency of Spec vs. Head feature specification.
(Poss. Nominals with Copula) The first clear
bit of evidence against any such wide-ranging Agr-based correlate
comes from Possessives with Copula Verbs. This stage represents
the onset of functional categories where IPs are seen to project
at will. Table 1.7 below illustrates that the specifications
of lower Object DPs are independent of the upper INFL:
i.e., both (higher) Subject/Gen D(P)s and (lower) Object/Gen
D(P)s can go either specified or underspecified (independently)
within an overall INFL-projection.
(26) Table 1.7
Poss. Noms with Copula Verb
Token Examples
IP constructions
a'. It is my t.v. It is your "e". That's
your car.
a. Correct Case:
n.=190 It's Nicolas'. It is a man's paper.
What's his-
b'. Him eye is broken. Him bike is broken.
b. Incorrect Case:
n.=40 Where's daddy bike? It is a boy
bike. It's-
dad turn. Daddy t.v. is broken.What's
him-
(26')

The ratio of incorrect-to-correct Possessive Case constructions
within an IP Copula phrase is approx. 1:5 (respectively) with
an overall 17% incorrect usage for required contexts. The percentage
may not seem large at first sight, but keep in mind that our
only task here is to dismiss any notion behind a wide-ranging
Agr correlation--nothing more. In fact, the 17% usage of incorrect
Poss. Noms. within IPs should by no means be cast as insignificant.
Next >>
[i] The reason why the suffix "s" only appears
with 3sg may have something to do with the notion that 3sg
is a default without person or number features (cf. Kayne:
89). In this sense, the suffix "s" is used
when items only carry tense (p.c Radford). This goes against
the notion (cf. note 12) that T and Agr may be fused together
in English (cf. Schütze: ibid) thus elevating any inherent
problems having to do with a fused T/Agr projection.
[ii] This suffix "s" +interpretable Tense
feature may be anchored in semantics and have nothing to do
with finiteness (as normally assumed). One possibility could
be to assign [+/-] Finiteness to Agreement and not Tense in
these early cases:
Tense => [+/-past]
Agreement =>
[+/- Finite], [1p,2p,3p], [-+/-plural],
This paves the way for a new
PRO account regarding Inflected Vs.
[iii] This is consistent with Radford's position (Radford:
1990) which maintains that Me subjects are Caseless
so will occur as subjects only when INFL is [-Agr]--other
uninterpretable Agr-features of I remain at LF.
[iv] I have only two example of [+T, -Agr] with an Objective
Case/Main verb (and not copula Is) in my entire corpus: Him
cries (File 25) and Me broke (File 25).
But see Huxley (1970), Aldridge (1988), Rispoli (1994c) for
such examples.
See also note 18 below.
[v] Out of a total of 82 unambiguous finite verbs (copulas)
marked by the suffix "s", 12 showed Acc case
(cf. Files 12-25).
[vi] It is noteworthy to point out that early distributional
contrast of Me vs. My for Subjects might suggest that
indeed some Case has been acquired for My. It is crucial
here to distinguish the use of My as an analogical Nominative
(see section on genitive errors below) from the use of My
as a Possessive Pronominal. The former case represents the
acquiring of Case while the latter usage of My (as
default) suggests there to be no Case). In this sense, Analogical
The along with Acc. Me and Poss. Nom. My/Mine
similarly share the likes of having a default case setting.
[vii] Radford (ibid.) citing Abney (1987) suggests that the
utterance My tiger book likewise might have the following
adult structure: [DP My/Mine [D e/ * 's][NP tiger book]] where
there is an empty allomorph of the determiner 's (phonetically
null) which assigns Genitive Case. In the child's utterance
(cf 17c) the allomorph would be grammatically null-hence,
the The-to-My analogy (viz., both possibly indicating
+Def only). The overgeneralization would then stem from the
empty allomorph being phonetically realized (as cited above
e.g., *Mine's..). Radford adds that such seemingly DPs as
My/Mine in early child speech are in fact imposters--i.e.,
though they look like adult versions of Possessives (acting
as a Spec of DP), they in fact function as simple Specifiers
of NP and haven't the same allomorph of Genitive Case ('s)
as granted in the adult structure.
[viii] An outstanding problem with the above DP-analysis is
that it would not account for Italian possessives like La
mia macchina (=The my car) where the possessive can't
be in Spec-DP but must be lower than DP (say e.g., PossP)
(p.c. Radford). Such problems however might be overcome if
we adopt Longobardi's analysis that mia here is adjectival
in nature. Moreover,
the above DP model (albeit problematic) suffices as an explanatory
aid to the account on offer.
Alternatively, Kayne (1994:
105) suggests that the Inflective properties of (case agreeing)
possessives should be more properly analyzed as IPs (and not
DPs): e.g., John's car = [D/Po [IP John ['s [car]]]].
|
|